GRACE, FALL, AND REDEMPTION: MENTAL ILLNESS AND OUR EVOLUTIONARY NARRATIVE *** By John Wylie *** The Montréal Review, July 2023 *** |
|||||
***
Embark with me on a journey delving deep into the relationship between severe mental illnesses and the primal emotions fueling our social interactions. These foundational emotional states, raw and instinctual, are the lifeblood of our connections with one another. However, when they malfunction, they can plunge us into the depths of severe mental illness. Contrary to popular belief, I suggest that these disorders do not twist or distort our emotions, but amplify them to a piercing intensity. This perspective suggests a profound implication: by studying the heightened symptoms of these illnesses, we can peer into the magnified mirror of our inherent social emotions, unlocking a clearer understanding of our shared human condition.
Social emotions have evolved to propel specific social functions across different stages of human evolution—from the primate epochs, through the times of our early human ancestors, to the age of Homo sapiens. I aim to forge empathetic bridges between the lived experiences of major mental illnesses and the distinct chapters of our social evolution. These connections will serve as guideposts as I weave diverse evolutionary science and theory into a comprehensive, causal narrative of the social emotions animating the human mind. The method employed here is not just philosophical but a basic means of communication found across cultures. Our brains are hardwired for storytelling. Neuroscientists Roger Sperry and Michael Gazzaniga even discovered a part of the brain they called the "interpreter," which makes sense of the world by weaving together different pieces of information into coherent narratives. This kind of thinking is part of our everyday lives as we continuously interpret and make sense of the world around us.
Guided by father of American psychology, William James's concept of “cash value,” the goal is to illuminate how these ideas can make a tangible difference. This evolutionary narrative may remind you of something—the age-old Abrahamic story of a fall from grace, followed by the promise of redemption. This suggests that perhaps these religious stories are rooted in our evolutionary past. And here's where the cash value of this article comes into the picture. Long stigmatized, but in this light, mental illnesses transform into beacons illuminating the magnificent journey through the ages undertaken by the feelings we all have for one another.
In the final sections, the evolutionary lens will add historical context to our current social drives. This is the key revelation: our capacity for virtue is not fleeting by. It is the heart of our humanity, sculpted by the relentless hand of natural selection over millions of years.
Within psychiatry, emotion is a vast landscape we continually traverse as it becomes disordered in mental illnesses. To understand how we arrived here, let's take a brief journey back in time. In the 19th Century, the prevailing belief was that mental illnesses resulted from complex brain disorders, which we optimistically anticipated comprehending fully one day. Then emerged the Freudian era, where focus shifted to our emotional experiences and their shaping by our childhood development. Freud viewed emotions not as fleeting responses but as constant drivers of social behavior, a perspective I will reflect using the term “emotions-and-motivations.”
In the early stage of my psychiatric career, I witnessed a shift from Freudian psychoanalysis to a biological approach, triggered by the serendipitous discovery of key prototype medications by the mid-80s. Views on mental illness transitioned towards “chemical imbalances” and genetic risks. Yet, despite decades of research into drug-brain interactions, a clear biological explanation for mental illnesses remains elusive.
Recent efforts to identify genetic links to mental illnesses have yielded little. A significant multi-center project based at Harvard painstakingly analyzed over a million genomes but failed to unearth any definitive genetic risk factors for any major mental illness. The researchers suggested improved diagnostic techniques might help, presuming mental illness to be largely a brain biology issue.
Understanding that severe mental illness fundamentally transpires at the level of our experiences, not just our microbiology, provides a more relatable frame for these conditions. Envision emotions like sounds—they can whisper or roar. Similar to the mechanism whereby an overpowering microphone feedback screech disrupts a tranquil atmosphere, mental illnesses, with their heightened emotional states, disrupt normal thought processes.
Unlike routine emotional struggles, major mental illnesses exhibit intense emotions that can override thought processes and shape beliefs. This theory posits that such emotions may become trapped in a feedback loop, intensifying excessively, a condition that medication could potentially rebalance. Consequently, mental illnesses can be viewed as amplified versions of our ordinary social emotions. According to evolutionary theory, these emotions have evolved sequentially over time. This lens reframes mental illnesses, not as mere "chemical imbalances" or "faulty genes," but rather profoundly resonate with our shared emotional heritage.
I propose that psychiatric conditions such as major depression and panic disorder might stem from a damaging emotional feedback loop within our minds. Interestingly, these disorders offer insights into the motivations that unite primate groups, including humans. In other words, the symptoms of these conditions could be considered vivid emotional relics, echoes from the earliest days of social behavior among primates.
Primates excel at navigating the complexities of group living, a capacity that evolved approximately 52 million years ago when our ancestors began to socialize. Their social prowess stems from two social fears that tempered antisocial fight-or-flight instincts and fostered interaction. Imagine four individuals in a strategic dance of alliances and rivalries, based on triangular “two-against-one” strategies—the basic molecule of politics. Through sustained interplay, the tension resolves, with one individual rising to the apex of a social pyramid, cementing bonds with the remaining three through a framework of dominance and submission. Within the broader group, these pyramids proliferate and interlock to form a complex social hierarchy. So, which social emotions enabled primates to moderate their fundamental fight-or-flight instincts and build complex social structures?
To adapt to group living, primates evolved two vital social fears: loss of personal bonds and fear of expulsion. These fears played the central role in forging and maintaining group cohesion. Initially, the fear of severing personal bonds encouraged family members to band together for protection against predators, a strategy that bolstered the collective survival of their shared genes. Over time, the benefits of this fear extended to all close relationships. In certain cases, we can observe this fear playing out in what's known as atypical depression, which can commonly be precipitated after losing someone close. Fear of loss can create a vicious cycle, with individuals retreating into relationship memories, amplifying their fear. In extreme cases, people with atypical depression may become convinced that suicide will reunite them with their lost loved one. Conversely, with melancholic depression, another severe form of depression, the motivation for suicide arises from a desperate need to escape feelings of entrapment.
Melancholic depression exemplifies how symptoms can mirror malfunctioning, deep-rooted emotions. At the heart of melancholia lies a profound fear of social banishment, frequently accompanied by unfounded beliefs of utter ruin or unforgivable transgressions. Interestingly, daily suffering is marked by feelings of entrapment and a preoccupation with escape. A poignant example from decades ago involves an individual suffering from melancholia who, tormented by intense feelings of confinement, dashed across a room, leapt through a window, and plummeted eight floors to his death. The emotion that malfunctions in melancholia usually propels us towards group cohesion by instilling a deep-seated fear of social banishment. Evolution cleverly adapted the instinctual urge to flee physical confinement, aligning it with the fear of ostracism to promote group unity. Essentially, the primitive impulse to flee confined spaces has been reengineered to be elicited by the abyss of banishment, thus responded to as a fearsome and confining wall—for the benefits of cohering groups. An evocative and insightful portrayal of melancholia is found in William Styron's short memoir, Darkness Visible (1990):
Panic disorder often emerges when someone feels trapped in a toxic relationship or unsatisfying job, feeling cornered but hesitant to cut ties. The condition frequently presents with a symptom normally elicited by physical entrapment—an overwhelming sensation of suffocation. In a bid to handle this unbearable state, individuals might desperately seek to create psychological distance, paradoxically leading to an equally frightful state of self-detachment or “depersonalization.” Panic disorder can be conceived as the terrifying cycle of rapid shifts between an intense psychological desire to escape sensations of suffocation and the alienating state of self-detachment. These oscillating states mirror our inherited primate social instincts: one urges us to maintain broader social connections, like an enclosing fence, and the other draws us into close relationships, like a magnet.
The shift from the ape mind to the human mind is the enduring mystery of human evolution. The transformation from the ape mind to the human mind presents an intriguing quandary. Uniquely human traits, such as collaboration, communication, and critical thinking, leave no physical trace. Esteemed developmental psychologist, Michael Tomasello, addresses this challenge by comparing the cognitive processes of apes and developing children, with a view to identifying the distinctively human components of our nature. Tomasello proposes that “collective intentionality”—a shared intent in communication—is a trait exclusive to humans, not apes. He suggests that this characteristic has genetic origins and isn't merely a cultural offshoot. His theory draws on child development studies from various cultures, emphasizing two major milestones. Around nine months of age, infants begin to display “shared intentionality”—a parent might point out a beautiful bird, and the infant is capable of engaging with this shared focus. By roughly three years of age, children start to express collective intentionality, voicing group norms and expectations, as in, “This is the correct way we do it.” Tomasello postulates that the aptitude for collective intentionality, essentially a shared purpose or authority, emerged due to its role in facilitating efficient collaborative foraging. This perspective implies that teamwork is the defining adaptation of humanity. He compares this evolution in our group behavior capacity to a key biological event—the "Cambrian Explosion"—when single-celled organisms evolved into complex multicellular life forms around 500 million years ago. This article supports Tomasello's claim that the transition from apes to humans was marked by a shift from individual to collective intentionality.
As individual cells came together to form complex multicellular organisms, individual apes similarly evolved into harmonious groups of early humans. Essential to this evolution is the distinction between cooperation—a mutual "win-win" situation—and coordination, a harmonious division of labor that solidly binds the group, signifying that everyone’s fortunes rise and fall together. This critical shift occurred in our evolutionary trajectory when the benefits of individual actions were overtaken by those of coordinated group behavior. Upon crossing this threshold, the productivity of teamwork took a quantum leap. The benefits to individuals, even cunning predators, were soon surpassed by advantages from strengthened inter-individual relationships. This process, where the advantages to relationships trump personal gains, is referred to as "Relational Selection."
Around half a billion years ago, the transition from solitary cells to coordinated multicellular organisms shifted the focus of natural selection from individual cells to their intercellular communication, leading to complex nervous systems. Mirroring this progression, natural selection in our own human lineage expanded its focus from individual apes to the communication systems within early human communities. This evolution of foundational forms of communication crafted our rich environment of human language, chosen specifically for its crucial role in facilitating collective coordination and action. This dialogue, continuously establishing shared norms and expectations, marks our collective evolutionary journey towards refined group coordination—the hallmark of our shared human lineage and the essence of our human nature. Tomasello argues that collective intentionality, which can be understood as group decision-making, is mostly influenced by cognitive empathy—a concept also known as Theory of Mind. This refers to our ability to understand and predict the thoughts of others, essentially a form of mental perspective-taking. However, this paper suggests a need to shift our focus. Instead of concentrating solely on the cognitive capacities of individuals for social cooperation, we should also consider the broader, more encompassing role of collective intentionality in shaping our understanding of the human mind.
What lies at the very heart of the mind? Consider life's building blocks—simple elements called amino acids—slowly acquiring the ability to make copies of themselves. This key feature is the spark that kindles life. Over time, small changes happen. Some of these changes turn out to be helpful, making it easier for those particular life forms to survive and create more copies of themselves. This process gradually instills the life forms with a core instinct to survive. It's like the first heartbeat of a mind. So, we can think of the mind as the will to survive that arises in the wake of natural selection. Let's now imagine the journey from single, independent cells to complex, interconnected networks of cells. During this transition, the fundamental drive for survival also evolved. It transcended the confines of individual cells, finding a new home within the relationships cells established with each other. This shift marked the birth of a more advanced form of “mind,” a collective survival instinct intertwined within the complex network of cellular connections, which gradually transformed into the neurological systems to coordinate the behavior of animals. Further down the evolutionary path, our ape ancestors evolved into humans with shared goals. At this stage, our survival instinct began another transformation as it expanded its focus beyond individual survival to strengthen relationships within early human groups. This shift sparked the emergence of a still more advanced form of human mind with the destiny to transform our legacy of aggression into an intricate web of communion and coordinated behavior.
Three key pieces of evidence illuminate the early evolution of humans (hominins). They suggest that these pioneering species began evolving into a unique social structure—a collective entity with increasing communication abilities that promoted teamwork and shared responsibilities. In short, the ecology of our collective mind shaped human evolution. Amid a severe decline in ape populations, the growing need for teamwork advantages led to a transformation. The focus moved from individual dominance to the productivity of collective authority, made possible by certain evolutionary changes.
As described, two basic fears are universal in all human societies—fear of losing interpersonal relationships and fear of banishment from groups. These fears, essential for maintaining unity in primate groups, were strengthened and their function extended in humans to ensure adherence to performance norms and ethical guidelines crucial for effective teamwork. This adaptation can be observed in the symptoms of obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), which is characterized by repeated actions and persistent thoughts. People with OCD often struggle with intense worry about incorrectly executing tasks or morally deficient actions and thoughts. This fear drives them to constantly review and redo their actions or rethink their thoughts, trapping them in an unending cycle. Deeper social emotions tied to morality, going beyond just task completion, enable effective participation in close-knit teamwork. I suggest that our moral compass stems from our evolutionary development of shared motivations to strive for justice.
The species in our own Homo genus evolved into proficient team hunters—a transition made possible by maintaining and refining innate responses towards fairness. The dominance-submission power struggle seen in apes evolved into a nuanced human dynamic—a symbiotic partnership between indignation at unfairness and guilt from wrongful actions—collectively suppressing individual dominance.
In certain contemporary hunter-gatherer societies, the evident principle of equality serves as a check against dominance. Such displays of justice are visible across diverse groups. There has been significant discourse around the evolution of justice and equality within the confines of game theory, which explores the dynamics of competitive interactions among self-interested individuals vying for scarce resources. It might be helpful, though, to momentarily deviate from this intricate theoretical discourse and seek a simpler, more direct understanding of the available evidence. The prominent Scottish economist and philosopher, Adam Smith, illuminated key insights in his groundbreaking publication, The Wealth of Nations (1776). He argued that societal wealth derives from a division of labor and self-interest, with the latter channeled into beneficial actions by an "invisible hand." However, in his prior work, The Theory of Moral Sentiments (1759), Smith underscored the absolute necessity of justice. He suggested that without justice, the intricate and broad construct of human society would instantly collapse.
Viewing Smith's insight through the lens of evolution, justice emerges as a fundamental quality for effective social interaction, appearing as a collective instinct honed over time to maximize the benefits of coordinating group behavior. Our triumph as a species isn't attributable to individual aptitude, but to our inherited aptitude for complex teamwork. Our deeply entrenched justice instincts, naturally selected across epochs, have empowered our unique human social system to thrive.
An intriguing testament to our Homo ancestors' collective intentionality lies in their toolmaking prowess. Take, for instance, the Acheulean handaxe. This tool, simple yet effective, emerged with our Homo genus and remarkably remained largely unaltered in design across continents for about 1.5 million years. This period coincided with significant human brain expansion, hinting at the growth of our collective consciousness.
Crafting these handaxes required genetic adaptations like manual dexterity, opposable thumb, and enhanced hand-eye coordination. However, the distinctive technique for creating these tools, as well as their specific form, must have been perpetuated through culture and learning. This raises a fascinating question. Typically, as cultural practices spread through imitation, they evolve rapidly, accumulating random changes. Darwin grappled with how dynamic change could take place within seemingly static species. However, the handaxe puzzle flips this on its head: how could a cultural tradition, usually quick to evolve, remain so stubbornly stable over vast stretches of time? In Fairweather Eden (1998), Michael Pitts and Mark Roberts studied stone chips from Boxgrove, England, thought to be remnants of toolmaking activities from half a million years ago. They identified these chips as fragments discarded during the creation of handaxes. From a meticulous analysis of the chip clusters' relative positions, Pitts and Roberts deduced that a small group of early hominins had gathered around a deceased horse, simultaneously forging their tools to butcher the animal.
In the Pleistocene epoch, characterized by extreme climate shifts and widespread human migration, early Homo societies refined their social frameworks. Collective authority and shared tasks intensified, further shaping societies that had long since moved beyond the dominance of individual members. Natural selection increasingly favored the productive collaboration of relationships over mere individual strength. In this context, interactions among migrating groups became more frequent and harmonious. The communal creation of handaxes, with their optimal teardrop design, not only served as a communal bonding ritual of parallel group labor, but also as the continual recreation of a powerful symbol of collective unity and cohesion among all human groups. This paints a poignant image of time immemorial, with the tradition of handaxe creation enduring through tens of thousands of generations. Enduring, timeless shared experiences provided stability amid the flux of individuals transitioning between groups, and the dissolving and re-emerging of bands. These shared sequences, these collective rhythms of everyday life, were threads that wove an enduring tapestry across entire continents, unbroken for over a million years.
Monogamy, the exclusive pairing of one male with one female, is significantly observed in bird species (85-95%), but rare in mammals (3-5%) and around 15% of primates. The shift towards monogamous social structures began approximately 16 million years ago among certain primate species, marking a departure from less constrained mating arrangements. Bernard Chapais (2013), in his seminal work on human social structures, posits that monogamy had a fundamental role in our evolution from apes to humans. He argues that the deep social connections between family groups and their associated in-law families, present in all contemporary human societies, are unique among primates. These group affiliations, he suggests, are rooted in monogamy, which he describes as a cornerstone of human uniqueness. In essence, our transition to monogamous relationships cultivated complex family structures with firm intergroup bonds, crucial in our evolution as a species. While modern humans don't adhere strictly to monogamy, it's plausible that a shift from promiscuity towards monogamy could have ignited the beginnings of collective behavior in our early hominin ancestors. Research reveals that among insects like ants and bees, exhibiting sophisticated collective behavior, the rise of monogamy aligned with the onset of their collective functioning (Hughes, et al., 2008). This parallel between humans and these insects hints at a potential pattern in the evolution of advanced collective behavior.
A notable correlation exists across the animal kingdom between monogamy and increased brain size. This relationship is attributed to the complex behaviors associated with monogamy, such as coordinated childcare, which demand more brain power than navigating a hierarchy (Dunbar & Shultz, 2007). As male competition for mates diminished over time, cooperative practices like shared "alloparenting" increased among our early hominin ancestors, as noted by Sarah Hrdy (2011). Initiated in the early hominin era and coming to full fruition in the Homo epoch, these shifts influenced all social interactions to reap the productive benefits of monogamous bonds more widely. Consequently, the necessity to coordinate survival strategies within groups spurred a notable brain expansion during the Homo era. Fundamentally, the frontal lobe, where most expansion occurred, was "colonized" by collective intentionality. This marked the pinnacle of our ancestors' evolution towards more complex collective abilities. Over six million years leading to Homo sapiens' emergence, evolution's driving force wasn't brute strength or individual prowess, but collective coordination and shared understanding's evolving grace. This radiant journey was not merely external, but intimately entwined within the minds of our ancestors, and echoed in the ongoing evolution of language—the neurological system that served as the nexus of our socially woven existence. Our ancestors, as collective custodians of wisdom and experience, engaged in persistent mental dialogue, collectively interpreting their surrounding world. In this Eden of collective cognition and dialogue, they traversed their journey through epochs, setting the foundation of humanity's trajectory. This was our ancestral state of grace before the approaching whisper of a fall.
In his groundbreaking work, The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex (1871), Charles Darwin proposed that attractiveness-enhancing traits, not just survival traits, significantly impacted human evolution. He used the peacock's majestic tail as a primary example, explaining that its primary function isn't survival but to captivate peahens—an illustration of sexual selection. Such displays, intended to attract potential mates and not directly related to survival, are common across the animal kingdom. Birds showcasing vibrant plumage or singing melodic tunes, insects producing distinctive chirps—all are means of announcing, "I'm here! . . . Are you nearby?"
Yale ornithologist Richard Prum, in The Evolution of Beauty (2017), builds on Darwin's controversial idea, arguing that sexual selection played a crucial role in Homo sapiens' evolution. Science need not prove that physical appeal and desirable behaviors often influence our mate selection. Here are some examples illustrating how sexual selection has significantly influenced human evolution:
What sparked this unique form of co-evolution in Homo sapiens, where a trait's appeal and attraction to it mutually influence each other? Sexual selection in our species isn't dependent on individual dominance, as seen in primate hierarchies, or the shared pursuit of fairness fundamental to teamwork. Rather, it's fueled by a person's aspiration to be admired by others. Vanity and narcissism, often framed as problematic or destructive tendencies in biblical parables and traditional Freudian psychoanalysis, play a key role in this process. The self-psychology movement, which emerged in the 1970s (Kohut, 1971), proposed a joy-oriented "self-system" distinct from the anxiety-driven "ego system." This self-system, initially kindled by the pleasure experienced from caregiver attention and approval in infancy, matures alongside the child, promoting a natural inclination to derive pleasure through social validation and countering the fear-induced restraints of the superego. This perspective casts vanity not as a vice, but as a fundamental prosocial motivating force, a concept even acknowledged by Adam Smith in The Theory of Moral Sentiments (1759):
Understanding vanity as a yearning for social validation, we can comprehend the evolution of sexual selection in Homo sapiens better. This process has broadened beyond the individual rewards of sexual desire to become what might be called "Social Selection." Social exhibition, characterized by elements of vanity or narcissism, extends beyond its primal function in courtship, playing a key role in all social relationships. This transition occurred as the individual rewards of sexual desire were surpassed and augmented by the collective benefits of social desire, which served to unify populations. Thus, our perpetual reciprocal allure, powered by our yearning for one another, has been favored by nature, cultivating extensive interwoven communities through its exceptional capacity for fostering social interaction. Emerging research employing ancient DNA techniques hints that Neanderthals, unlike our species, tended towards inbreeding and maintained static social structures. The genetic material of a Neanderthal woman shows traces of close-relative mating, a pattern mirrored in the genetic information from thirteen other Neanderthal individuals (Prüfer et al., 2017; Skov et al., 2022). Contrasting sharply with this, the genomes of Homo sapiens from 34,000 years ago demonstrate low levels of interrelatedness within living groups, implying a more expansive mating network akin to the structure observed in present-day hunter-gatherer societies (Sikora et al., 2017). Additionally, another examination of ancient genomes uncovers geographically widespread mating, mobility, and population blending across Africa 50,000 years ago, with hints of population structuring emerging around 20,000 years ago (Lipson et al., 2022).
The key strength of large, interconnected communities is their capacity to amass and preserve a vast reservoir of knowledge. This accumulated wisdom accelerates cultural evolution, fostering innovation in tools, survival strategies, and dwellings. This collective knowledge is not static; it's dynamically shared and enriched within the community, fostering an evolving understanding. Anthropologist Robert Boyd encapsulates this idea, positing that “perhaps our intricate culture doesn't emanate from solitary cogitation, but from the communal knowledge we amass collectively in groups” (Culotta, 2010, p. 164). Additionally, this process sets in motion a cascade of cultural evolution influencing the development of our economic, political, and artistic pursuits, all driven by the quest for social recognition through a diverse range of public displays. The intense rise of cultural evolution began around 40,000 years ago, aligning with the era of cave art. However, advancements in stone tool fabrication that exceeded the capabilities demonstrated in the Acheulean handaxe, are linked to the earliest Homo sapiens fossils (Richter et al., 2017). This cultural selection of knowledge, beauty—and of truth—heralds a remarkable triumph for our species. Perhaps, it's one of the most pivotal milestones in the chronicle of life. Even in our intricate, modern society, we find resonance in the ancient wisdom of Ecclesiastes 2:11: “Then I looked on all the works that my hands had wrought, and on the labour that I had laboured to do: and, behold, all was vanity and vexation of spirit . . .” This human quest for recognition, much like a peacock showing off its vibrant tail, has been the key motivator in forming our shared culture and fueling our economies, as noted by Adam Smith. Yet, this drive has a double edge. When it evolves into a fierce competition, problems arise. Our yearning for recognition has directed its own evolution. Ironically, while this drive projects our communal power into a new realm, it also suggests potential pitfalls and future challenges.
Freud characterized social emotions as persistent motivational states. Viewed evolutionarily, these emotions seem favored by natural selection due to the sustained benefits of their prolonged intensity. Simultaneously, co-evolving control mechanisms at the neurobiological level have modulated this intensity. However, at the statistical fringes of populations, these regulatory systems may falter under severe emotional duress. Consequently, social emotions, left unchecked, could spiral into mental disorders via runaway positive feedback loops, akin to processes observed in diseases such as cancer. Consider the manic phase of bipolar disorder—a manifestation of the powerful emotions and impulses that drive our species' “new mind.” Pathological mania might be viewed as a “cost of doing business”—an unforeseen side effect of our cultural and economic advantages. Similarly, disorders such as major depression and panic disorder might be seen as trade-offs for the security inherent in communal living. Meanwhile, the emergence of obsessive-compulsive disorder could be interpreted as a byproduct of our capacity for collective functioning. During a manic episode, pleasure—which usually fuels our quest for social approval—surges wildly, sparking a whirlwind of hyperactive, euphoric actions. Individuals caught in this manic state become both enchanting performers and their own captive audience, caught in a destructive feedback loop. One symptom of the manic phase of bipolar disorder suggests the recent evolution of our complex drive for social admiration. As a physician, I've occasionally found my objective stance shaken by the linguistic dexterity displayed by a patient enveloped in mania. A times, their speech unfurls as a torrent of eloquent expressions, punctuated with rhetorical flourishes and a compelling allure. The meanings they craft can spark a surge of exceptionally inventive narrative thinking. This linguistic spectacle does more than mirror the ostentatious displays found in nature, such as the peacock's tail; it transcends into our distinctly human sphere of social exhibition. Sylvia Nasar, in her biography of mathematician John Nash, "A Beautiful Mind" (1998), captures such a moment during a visit to McLean Hospital in Boston:
We could consider mental illnesses as emotional fossils. As pinpointing a physical fossil's age requires careful analysis of surrounding geological layers, understanding our vanity's emergence—as reflected in mania—needs to be placed within our cognitive evolution timeline. The capacity for elaborate linguistic performances, exemplified by Lowell, seems to have emerged fairly recently, suggesting our drive for esteem arose within our own species' history. Moreover, viewing human language evolutionarily uncovers how our new mind (“me”)—with its fluctuating and ostentatious tendencies—creates fascinating verbal displays within flexible, responsive grammatical structures. These rules are firmly anchored by our ancestral mind's stable, communal intent ("we").
A significant byproduct of our species' persistent desire for desirability is the gradual rise of both individual and collective violence, or war. Anthropologists Nam Kim and Marc Kissel, in their work Emergent Warfare in Our Evolutionary Past (2018), put forth evidence that warfare has gradually developed throughout our species' existence—approximately 300,000 years—with clear signs of recurrent warfare emerging after the last glacial maximum. However, evidence of intra-species violence among our hominin ancestors is relatively scarce—cannibalism, yes, but not widespread violence. Hence, pathological mania constitutes only a small portion of the negative impacts derived from our fervent, competitive yearnings for one another. Anthropologist Christopher Boehm has noted that in contemporary human hunter-gatherer societies, "the most severe conflicts often arise from poorly socially regulated competition over females" (Boehm, 2012, p. 846). In today's society, unsolved murder cases frequently point to the spouse as the primary suspect, with motives most commonly rooted in greed and romantic rivalry. Could the pressures imposed by countless generations of individuals in love, attempting to unite diverse tribes, have unintentionally ignited clannish hostilities reminiscent of Shakespeare's Montagues and Capulets?
Charisma, a personality attribute honed by sexual and social selection, considerably influences the shaping of modern societies. Authors David Graeber and David Wengrow, in their iconoclastic work, The Dawn of Everything (2021), underscore the pivotal role that charismatic leaders played in the rise of dynastic cultures across Mesopotamia, Egypt, and later in the Americas, including the Inca and Aztec civilizations. Initially, these leaders used their charm and allure to gather substantial followings. As their influence grew, they began to demonstrate their might through ritualistic displays of murder, striking fear into their followers. In time, they constructed monumental edifices to exalt themselves and pay homage to the formidable gods they claimed intimate affiliation with. Primates, including humans, possess an innate aptitude for navigating intricate social hierarchies. Among our hominin predecessors, patterns of dominance and submission evolved into collective deference towards consensual social authority. However, in Homo sapiens, this deep-seated communal authority shifted from being relationship-based to being dominated by authoritarian tribal hierarchies. In this new dynamic, individuals' intense aspirations for prestige and admiration intertwined with their audience's emerging inclination to idolize them. This synergy of passionate desires could ignite a cyclonic flurry of escalating emotions and ambitions, a turbulent vortex potentially spiraling into exaggerated assertions of dominance and the dawn of despotism. The emergence of dynasties signified a significant change in the dynamics of early post-nomadic societal structures. Interestingly, dynasties arose on the outskirts of far-flung settlements without archeological evidence of centralized government, administration, or an elite class (Graeber and Wengrow, 2021, p. 289). This ascent of dynasties forms part of a broader narrative that illustrates our species' descent into a cyclical state of warfare, a topic we will revisit later. Central to this transformation is the powerful emotional contagion of charisma and desire. As a result, battlefield selection favoring warfare-proficient societies has significantly influenced the evolutionary prelude to our species' current era. These "heroic" societies have leveraged their inherent propensity for teamwork, developing war tactics and societal structures to establish competitive dominance hierarchies. This selection for superiority in conflict has driven these societies to evolve into increasingly militaristic entities.
The concept of group selection has a fraught history, having been used as a rationale for Nazi ideology, and current attitudes toward group selection are that it is weak evolutionary force in the game theory of competing individuals. Charles Darwin proposed his notion of group selection, possibly as a way to cast his contentious theory in a favorable light by emphasizing the selection of traits beneficial to the group. His use of the term "victorious" warrants particular attention:
In this article’s perspective, warfare also facilitates the shift of collective intentionality—redirecting the beneficiary of selection from justice and productive relationships to competition and group fitness. Throughout our species' archaeologically documented history, instincts originally evolved for the substantial benefits of cooperative interaction have been perpetually redirected towards the benefits of tribal conflict.
Homo sapiens, a species evolved with a deep-seated affinity for authority aimed at coordinating relationships for collective benefit, now grapples with the rise of non-consensual, authoritarian hierarchies. These contrast with the organic, consensus-based hierarchies of our ancestors, where roles naturally emerged to best serve the group's interests. I propose this shift reveals adaptations in our highly complex genetic makeup that govern collective emotions-and-motivations, and which are sensitive to our species’ shifting selection pressures. These non-consensual, authoritarian hierarchies, while bringing efficiency and order —critical elements for success in tribal warfare— exploit our innate cooperative behavior, magnifying collective combat prowess and economic dominance. This conflates submission and obedience, a fusion further intensified by charismatic figures demanding loyalty, effectively reshaping authority in an “us-versus-them” context. In current society, the impact of this collective evolutionary shift is manifested in divisions and biases throughout society. These divisions and biases materialize in social assemblages at every scale, from intimate gatherings to vast political blocs, influencing all aspects of communal interaction—including the scientific community. Today's political divide reflects a tension between the efficiency of political and economic dominance hierarchies and the humanity inherent in the relational skills that are the root source of their power.
Throughout our recent evolution, Homo sapiens developed an intuitive "social navigation system" to interpret subtle authority signals. This system engenders a powerful allegiance, or “belief in,” the norms and biases of the groups with which we identify. This subconscious process, however, is impaired in individuals with schizophrenia. These individuals often experience marginalization as their emotional capacity to discern the competitive interplay between self and others, or in-group and out-group, deteriorates. Embedded within the intricate exchanges of human conversation lies a subconscious layer of personal and group political strategizing—a dynamic that becomes dysfunctional in the context of schizophrenia.
The frequent coexistence of mania and schizophrenia may result from the intertwined dynamics of social display and group-level identity—recent evolutionary phenomena. Mania and schizophrenia can be viewed as byproducts of the two fundamental aspects of our modern minds: one propelled by individual ambition, a driving force behind our economies, and the other fueled by unwavering loyalty to specific groups. These two interwoven instincts—one born of personal desires and aspirations, and the other emerging from our allegiance to distinct group entities—have been favored by natural selection for their persistent intensity. However, in some individuals, these mechanisms can malfunction, resulting in the symptoms of mania and schizophrenia.
The "direction" of intentionality in mania and schizophrenia offers insightful contrasts. In mania, the characteristic symptoms manifest as an expansive outpouring of thoughts and words, originating from the individual and directed towards society. Schizophrenia, however, is marked by disjointed thoughts and auditory hallucinations seemingly originating from society and targeting the individual. Again, in the analogy with audio systems, mania is like the output of a loudspeaker and schizophrenia to the input of a microphone in a feedback screech scenario: one echoes a manic outpouring from self, the other a schizophrenic inpouring from society.
Consider the 2013 Navy Yard shootings in Washington, DC for illustration. Initial hypotheses labeled the perpetrator as a terrorist, driven by adversarial group ideologies. Subsequent investigations, however, revealed his struggle with schizophrenia. An email he had authored, later disclosed by the FBI, illuminated his perceived motivation: "Ultra-low frequency [microwave] attack is what I've been subjected to for the last three months, and to be perfectly honest, that is what has driven me to this" (Botelho and Sterling, 2013). In schizophrenia, typical group-level narratives—usually met with submission, obedience, and belief—can be perceived as forceful barrages. This happens when the instinctive emotional mechanism subtly conveying group authority to its members malfunctions, culminating in an experience of excessive amplification.
Recurrent warfare emerged with humanity's shift from nomadic hunting-gathering to a sedentary lifestyle, driven by agricultural advancements and expanding trade networks. These transformations fostered wealth accumulation, necessitating protection and sparking endless cycles of warfare. A disturbing parallel can be drawn with successful eusocial insect species, particularly ants, where societal interactions are dictated by pheromones—the insect equivalent of human social emotions. Edward O. Wilson, sociobiology's founder, posits in The Social Conquest of Earth (2012) that ants transitioned to group selection with the construction of defensible nests, inciting ceaseless warfare. The resulting chronic warfare has culminated in their dystopian social systems characterized by sterile worker/warrior castes and queens, with the intensity of defense proportional to the complexity and size of the nest (Wilson, 2012, p.130). This analogy prompts a disquieting question: are we, humans, embarking on a similar path?
Reflecting on our species' evolutionary trajectory, we must remember that these disruptive instincts, despite their substantial influence, are recent additions in our evolutionary narrative and are still balancing with our primal human instincts for justice. Two essential goals emerge from this perspective: first, to channel the potent desire for recognition unique to Homo sapiens, forming integral part in our human narrative; second, to firmly guide resurgent primate dominance hierarchies towards promoting economic prosperity.
Renowned psychiatrist-philosopher Karl Jaspers identified a significant period known as the “Axial Age.” Peaking around 500 BCE, this epoch was remarkable for the concurrent and independent emergence of “the spiritual foundations of humanity... in China, India, Persia, Judea, and Greece...” (Jaspers, 1951, p. 98). The Axial Age witnessed a renaissance of ingrained collective instincts for justice and morality, overshadowed by eras of group selection favoring militaristic perspectives.
The steady development of legal systems—from Hebrew and Roman law to English Common Law and the U.S. Constitution—signified the transition from a state of near constant warfare to more civil forms of exchange. Though political and economic transactions echo primate dominance contests, they're moderated by human-made laws and regulations. Modern political philosophies emphasize freedom, especially economic freedom, against tyranny's constraints. The motivation animating both economic competition and tyranny has very recently sprung from the same well of our fervent desire for one another, and the leveraged power of both derives from a legacy of six million years of evolving to function collectively as if inhabiting a single mind. Justice evolved to enable us to live our lives within this shared public space, our ecological “niche” where human lives have always flourished. Without instincts for justice ingrained into our relationships, we might still be secluded in our forest refuges, engaging in boundless freedom to compete for dominance within our extended families, yet utterly lacking the ability to produce goods or services of any economic worth. As poet William Butler Yeats observed, “We taste and feel and see the truth. We do not reason ourselves into it” (Yeats, 2013, p. 195).1 The Enlightenment era marked the re-emergence of our innate collective instinct to revere the authority of truth—an instinct that held paramount significance within the numerous tribes of our ancestral species. The individuals within these tribes spent their entire lives immersed in passionate discourse aimed at discerning the most equitable and correct shared course forward, effectively functioning as a single creature.
This essay traverses the landscape of two distinct human evolutionary forces, one propelled by fear and trembling, the other by pleasure and joy. To illuminate their import, I invoke the enduring narrative of the Garden of Eden. These animations represent divergent pathways, echoing the choices set before Adam and Eve. One pathway, bathed in the grace of communion, fosters collective well-being, peace, and harmony. The other, tempting the individual with the allure of fulfilling desires, bears the resemblance of a Faustian bargain. Through this lens, our evolutionary journey unfolds as an epic biblical pilgrimage, wherein our understanding of humanity's current plight is recast.
Tracing the intricate threads of our evolution illuminates poignant resonances with the experience of mental illness. This dimension casts a deep, empathic light upon our shared evolutionary saga—a felt echo of an epic tale spun by humanity itself. This dimension deepens an understanding of our shared journey, answering Edward O. Wilson's call for a “new mythos,” firmly grounded in truth:
Delving into our story's profundities, we echo Joseph Campbell: “We are this beautiful planet's children... its eyes and mind, its seeing and its thinking”— and I add “feeling.” We have not been sculpted by the hands of a divine essence, but have rather emerged from the womb of this resplendent planet. Our ascent to collective consciousness is an ongoing odyssey, an unfinished symphony, whose ultimate fate is the province of faith, prayer, and patience. Our choices, their consequences, and our interpretation of them, will continue to braid an intricate weaving of our humanity’s emotional experiences. Our destiny, steadfast in its course, will unfold into as yet unrealized dimensions, awaiting our voyage to the far horizon of our cherished deep blue orb. ***
*** ***
***
*** |